Five-Year-Old Children’s Working Memory Can Be Improved When Children Act On A Transparent Goal Cue

被引:0
|
作者
Christophe Fitamen
Agnès Blaye
Valérie Camos
机构
[1] Université de Fribourg,
[2] Université d’Aix-Marseille & CNRS-LPC,undefined
来源
关键词
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Working memory is a key component of human cognition and its development throughout childhood a major predictor of cognitive development and school achievement. Noticeably, preschoolers exhibit poor performance in working memory tasks. The present study aimed at testing different means to improve working memory performance in preschoolers. To this aim, we tested the effect of abstract and transparent goal cues in a Brown-Peterson task performed by 4- and 5-year-old preschoolers. If the transparent goal cue helps to better maintain the instructions, it should lead to better memory performance. Moreover, preschoolers had to track, either visually or with their fingers, the goal cue during the retention interval. If the motor activity favors the active engagement of the children in the task, the finger tracking should lead to improvement in memory performance. Our findings were that 5-year-old children benefitted from a transparent goal cue when they acted on it, while 4-year-old children did not show any improvement. These results suggest that working memory performance can be improved in 5-year-old children when the task embeds elements that can scaffold the task goal.
引用
收藏
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [21] Insulin resistance and its metabolic impact in five-year-old children
    Voss, LD
    Wilkin, TJ
    Howdle, SM
    Jeffery, AN
    Metcalf, BS
    Murphy, MM
    DIABETOLOGIA, 2001, 44 : A255 - A255
  • [22] The role of parents' distancing strategies in the development of five-year-old children's Theory of Mind
    Galende, Nuria
    Sanchez de Miguel, Manuel
    Arranz, Enrique
    EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CARE, 2012, 182 (02) : 207 - 220
  • [23] Four- and five-year-old children's views and perceptions related to their teachers' expectations
    Cooper, Maria
    McDonald, Lyn
    Rubie-Davies, Christine
    CURRICULUM MATTERS, 2021, 17 : 5 - 23
  • [24] VARIATIONS IN PERCEPTIONS OF FIVE-YEAR-OLD CHILDREN'S SCHOOL READINESS AMONG PARENTS AND TEACHERS
    Pitsia, V
    Kent, G.
    11TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF EDUCATION, RESEARCH AND INNOVATION (ICERI2018), 2018, : 8426 - 8435
  • [25] the realism and sex type of four- to five-year-old children's occupational aspirations
    Care, Esther
    Deans, Jan
    Brown, Robert
    JOURNAL OF EARLY CHILDHOOD RESEARCH, 2007, 5 (02) : 155 - 168
  • [26] Object and action picture naming in three- and five-year-old children
    Masterson, Jackie
    Druks, Judit
    Gallienne, Donna
    JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE, 2008, 35 (02) : 373 - 402
  • [27] Goal Neglect and Working Memory Capacity in 4-to 6-Year-Old Children
    Marcovitch, Stuart
    Boseovski, Janet J.
    Kane, Michael J.
    Knapp, Robin J.
    CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 2010, 81 (06) : 1687 - 1695
  • [28] Five-year-old children with autism spectrum disorders struggle with disengaging attention
    Wang, Wei
    Cheng, Chun
    Xu, Zhiyu
    Xue, Licheng
    Fu, Wanlu
    Zhao, Jing
    COGNITIVE PROCESSING, 2025,
  • [29] Caries in five-year-old children and associations with family-related factors
    Mattila, ML
    Rautava, P
    Sillanpää, M
    Paunio, P
    JOURNAL OF DENTAL RESEARCH, 2000, 79 (03) : 875 - 881
  • [30] An Investigation Of The Spontaneous Speech Five-Year-Old Deaf-Born Children
    Carr, Josephine
    JOURNAL OF SPEECH AND HEARING DISORDERS, 1953, 18 (01): : 22 - 29