INFLUENCE OF MALE COMPETITION AND FEMALE MATE CHOICE ON MALE MATING SUCCESS IN BARBARY MACAQUES (MACACA-SYLVANUS)

被引:59
|
作者
KUESTER, J
PAUL, A
机构
关键词
D O I
10.1163/156853992X00606
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
To evaluate the importance of male competition and female mate choice for male mating success in Barbary macaques, focal female observations during the conceptional estrus were conducted in a large semifree-ranging group. Analysis of sexual behaviour included 121.7 h of observation of 19 focal females. In addition, ad libitum recorded male agonistic interactions, occurring in the vicinity of the focal females, were analyzed. Both sexes initiated sexual associations, and females were found almost always in contact (within 2 m) with a male. Most matings took place within 1 min after contact establishment, and the proportion of these quick matings was especially high for subadult males, which mainly "sneaked" copulations during moments of distraction of adult males. Mating contacts were longer than non-mating contacts, and varied in duration from a few seconds to more than 2 hours. Mating contacts with adult males did not differ in length with respect of the initiating sex. Females were considerably more active in terminating than initiating contacts. Females mated, on average, once every 30 min, and had 1-10 different partners (out of 37 sexually mature males) during a 4 h observation session. Females mated with 40-100% of their contact partners. An absence of mating with specific males was due to interference by other males, improper timing of contact, or (temporary) lack of attractivity of the female rather than related with a rejection of these males in almost all cases. Similarly, a highly significant positive correlation between mating frequency of a male and time spent in the vicinity of the focal females revealed that females did not discriminate among potential mates, and, hence, did not exercise mate choice. The majority of matings (71%) were accumulated by 7 out of the 9 oldest males and additionally 2 young adults. One indicator for sexual competition among males was the peak of male injuries during the mating season. Aggressive interactions between adult and subadult males, indicating a clear-cut dominance of the adults, occurred frequently, while dyadic agonistic interactions between adult males were rare and inconsistent. The available data indicated age-inversed rank relations and were not predictive for mating success. A highly significant positive correlation was, however, found between male mating success and the participation as ally in polyadic agonistic interactions. The oldest males gave and received most support and were rarely victims of coalitions while the reverse was found for young adult males. All males followed an "age rule", after which the older of 2 males was supported during a conflict. Consequently, male power asymmetry in polyadic conflicts ran counter that in dyadic situations, and could change quickly depending on the presence of potential allies. Chances for dyadic solutions of conflicts were rare on the ground where most estrous females and the old males spent their time. Although females did not reject potential mates, they nevertheless influenced male mating success by inciting male competition. Females often tried to contact a new partner after a mating, thereby actively putting both males into conflict. The creation of such encounters was possible only between males with low power asymmetry, and only males which got successfully through these frequent female-initiated tests of their power had a high mating success. Incitation of male competition was discussed as a female mating tactic in species with a high sexual dimorphism. Compared with other macaques, the Barbary macaque belongs to such species.
引用
收藏
页码:192 / 217
页数:26
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] INTERGROUP INTERACTIONS IN WILD BARBARY MACAQUES (MACACA-SYLVANUS), GHOMARAN RIF MOUNTAINS, MOROCCO
    MEHLMAN, PT
    PARKHILL, RS
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 1988, 15 (01) : 31 - 44
  • [43] Do female copulation calls influence mating outcome?: A study in free-ranging Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus)
    Pfefferle, Dana
    Brauch, Katrin
    Heistermann, Michael
    Hodges, Keith
    Fischer, Julia
    FOLIA PRIMATOLOGICA, 2008, 79 (05) : 371 - 371
  • [44] Male Dominance Rank, Female Mate Choice and Male Mating and Reproductive Success in Captive Chimpanzees
    Ekaterina Klinkova
    J. Keith Hodges
    Kerstin Fuhrmann
    Tom de Jong
    Michael Heistermann
    International Journal of Primatology, 2005, 26 : 357 - 484
  • [45] Male dominance rank, female mate choice and male mating and reproductive success in captive chimpanzees
    Klinkova, E
    Hodges, JK
    Fuhrmann, K
    de Jong, T
    Heistermann, M
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 2005, 26 (02) : 357 - 384
  • [46] SOCIAL CLIMBER - INDEPENDENT RISE IN RANK BY A FEMALE BARBARY MACAQUE (MACACA-SYLVANUS)
    SMALL, MF
    FOLIA PRIMATOLOGICA, 1990, 55 (02) : 85 - 91
  • [47] TRIADIC INTERACTIONS IN CAPTIVE BARBARY MACAQUES (MACACA-SYLVANUS, LINNAEUS, 1758) - AGONISTIC BUFFERING
    SMITH, EO
    PEFFERSMITH, PG
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 1982, 2 (01) : 99 - 107
  • [48] Sexual selection in Japanese macaques .2. female mate choice and male-male competition
    Soltis, J
    Mitsunaga, F
    Shimizu, K
    Nozaki, M
    Yanagihara, Y
    DomingoRoura, X
    Takenaka, O
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1997, 54 : 737 - 746
  • [49] The Effect of Dominance Rank on the Distribution of Different Types of Male-Infant-Male Interactions in Barbary Macaques (Macaca sylvanus)
    Kubenova, Barbora
    Ostner, Julia
    Schuelke, Oliver
    Majolo, Bonaventura
    Smilauer, Petr
    Konecna, Martina
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 2019, 40 (03) : 300 - 315
  • [50] Male coalitions and female behaviour affect male mating success independent of dominance rank and female receptive synchrony in wild Barbary macaques
    Christopher Young
    Sabine Hähndel
    Bonaventura Majolo
    Oliver Schülke
    Julia Ostner
    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2013, 67 : 1665 - 1677