A BEHAVIORAL MEASURE OF HAND PREFERENCE AS OPPOSED TO HAND SKILL

被引:56
|
作者
BRYDEN, MP
SINGH, M
STEENHUIS, RE
CLARKSON, KL
机构
[1] MEERUT UNIV,MEERUT 250001,UTTAR PRADESH,INDIA
[2] UNIV WESTERN ONTARIO HOSP,LONDON N6A 5A5,ON,CANADA
关键词
D O I
10.1016/0028-3932(94)90048-5
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Twenty-five self-professed left-handers and 21 self-professed right-handers were given a variety of performance tests to assess handedness, along with a preference inventory and a dichotic listening test of language lateralization. The performance tests included the Annett pegboard task, the Tapley and Bryden dot-filling task, and two procedures, the long pegboard and long dots tasks, that were intended to assess the point in space at which a particular unimanual movement became sufficiently awkward for one to shift to the other hand. All four of these performance tests differentiated between left-handers and right-handers, although the differences between handedness groups were somewhat larger when handedness was defined in terms of the preference inventory rather than on the basis of self-report. When the difference between preferred and non-preferred hands was examined, the best predictor of hand preference was the long pegboard task, Such a finding is consistent with the view that the long pegboard provides a behavioral measure of hand preference, while the pegs and dots tasks are more closely linked to specific skills. In addition, the correlations between individual preference items and the dichotic right-ear advantage suggest that language lateralization is related to rather different handedness measures than those usually used to define handedness. This finding would suggest that handedness and language lateralization are determined by somewhat different mechanisms.
引用
收藏
页码:991 / 999
页数:9
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Methodology of motor skill and laterality:: New test of hand preference in Macaca nemestrina
    Rigamonti, MM
    Previde, EP
    Poli, MD
    Marchant, LF
    McGrew, WC
    [J]. CORTEX, 1998, 34 (05) : 693 - 705
  • [32] HAND PREFERENCE AND SKILL IN 115 CHILDREN OF 2 LEFT-HANDED PARENTS
    ANNETT, M
    [J]. BRITISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 1983, 74 (FEB) : 17 - 32
  • [33] HAND PREFERENCE IN AIRCREW
    GEYDE, JL
    [J]. AEROSPACE MEDICINE, 1963, 34 (03): : 255 - &
  • [34] HAND PREFERENCE IN INDIA
    MANDAL, MK
    PANDEY, G
    SINGH, SK
    ASTHANA, HS
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 1992, 27 (06) : 433 - 442
  • [35] Hand Preference in the Deaf
    Manas K. Mandal
    H. S. Asthana
    C. B. Dwivedi
    M. P. Bryden
    [J]. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 1999, 11 : 265 - 273
  • [36] LEMUR HAND PREFERENCE
    MILLIKEN, G
    FORSYTHE, C
    WARD, J
    [J]. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 1986, 10 (04) : 420 - 420
  • [37] Task difficulty in a performance-based method measure of hand preference
    Bryden, PJ
    Roy, EA
    Cavill, S
    [J]. JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 24 : 38 - 39
  • [38] Hand preference in the deaf
    Mandal, MK
    Asthana, HS
    Dwivedi, CB
    Bryden, MP
    [J]. JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL AND PHYSICAL DISABILITIES, 1999, 11 (03) : 265 - 273
  • [39] The Flinders Handedness survey (FLANDERS): A brief measure of skilled hand preference
    Nicholls, Michael E. R.
    Thomas, Nicole A.
    Loetscher, Tobias
    Grimshaw, Gina M.
    [J]. CORTEX, 2013, 49 (10) : 2914 - 2926
  • [40] Hand preference, sexual preference, and transsexualism
    Green, R
    Young, R
    [J]. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR, 2001, 30 (06) : 565 - 574