Following instructions from working memory: Why does action at encoding and recall help?

被引:0
|
作者
Agnieszka J. Jaroslawska
Susan E. Gathercole
Richard J. Allen
Joni Holmes
机构
[1] Queen’s University Belfast,School of Psychology
[2] MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit,School of Psychology
[3] University of Leeds,undefined
来源
Memory & Cognition | 2016年 / 44卷
关键词
Working memory; Following instructions; Enactment; Self-performed task; Action advantage;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Two experiments investigated the consequences of action at encoding and recall on the ability to follow sequences of instructions. Children ages 7–9 years recalled sequences of spoken action commands under presentation and recall conditions that either did or did not involve their physical performance. In both experiments, recall was enhanced by carrying out the instructions as they were being initially presented and also by performing them at recall. In contrast, the accuracy of instruction-following did not improve above spoken presentation alone, either when the instructions were silently read or heard by the child (Experiment 1), or when the child repeated the spoken instructions as they were presented (Experiment 2). These findings suggest that the enactment advantage at presentation does not simply reflect a general benefit of a dual exposure to instructions, and that it is not a result of their self-production at presentation. The benefits of action-based recall were reduced following enactment during presentation, suggesting that the positive effects of action at encoding and recall may have a common origin. It is proposed that the benefits of physical movement arise from the existence of a short-term motor store that maintains the temporal, spatial, and motoric features of either planned or already executed actions.
引用
收藏
页码:1183 / 1191
页数:8
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] Delayed Recall and Working Memory MMSE Domains Predict Delirium following Cardiac Surgery
    Price, Catherine C.
    Garvan, Cynthia
    Hizel, Loren P.
    Lopez, Marcos G.
    Billings, Frederic T.
    JOURNAL OF ALZHEIMERS DISEASE, 2017, 59 (03) : 1027 - 1035
  • [42] The Recall of Information from Working Memory Insights from Behavioural and Chronometric Perspectives
    Towse, John N.
    Cowan, Nelson
    Hitch, Graham J.
    Horton, Neil J.
    EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2008, 55 (06) : 371 - 383
  • [43] Affective valence of words impacts recall from auditory working memory
    Gotoh, Fumiko
    JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 24 (02) : 117 - 124
  • [44] Does visuo-spatial working memory generally contribute to immediate serial letter recall?
    Fuerstenberg, A.
    Rummer, R.
    Schweppe, J.
    MEMORY, 2013, 21 (06) : 722 - 731
  • [46] Following instructions in a dual-task paradigm: Evidence for a temporary motor store in working memory
    Jaroslawska, Agnieszka J.
    Gathercole, Susan E.
    Holmes, Joni
    QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2018, 71 (11): : 2439 - 2449
  • [47] Does perceptual grouping improve visuospatial working memory? Optimized processing or encoding bias
    Antonio Prieto
    Vanesa Peinado
    Julia Mayas
    Psychological Research, 2022, 86 : 1297 - 1309
  • [48] Does perceptual grouping improve visuospatial working memory? Optimized processing or encoding bias
    Prieto, Antonio
    Peinado, Vanesa
    Mayas, Julia
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2022, 86 (04): : 1297 - 1309
  • [49] Why are difficult figural matrices hard to solve? The role of selective encoding and working memory capacity
    Krieger, Florian
    Zimmer, Hubert D.
    Greiff, Samuel
    Spinath, Frank M.
    Becker, Nicolas
    INTELLIGENCE, 2019, 72 : 35 - 48
  • [50] SIMILARITY OF ENCODING CONTEXT DOES NOT INFLUENCE RESISTANCE TO MEMORY IMPAIRMENT FOLLOWING MISINFORMATION
    BOWMAN, LL
    ZARAGOZA, MS
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 1989, 102 (02): : 249 - 264