Gelada vocal sequences follow Menzerath's linguistic law

被引:55
|
作者
Gustison, Morgan L. [1 ]
Semple, Stuart [2 ]
Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon [3 ]
Bergman, Thore J. [1 ,4 ]
机构
[1] Univ Michigan, Dept Psychol, 580 Union Dr, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA
[2] Univ Roehampton, Ctr Res Evolutionary Social & Interdisciplinary A, London SW15 4JD, England
[3] Univ Politecn Cataluna, Dept Ciencies Comp, Lab Relat Algorithm Complex & Learning Res Grp, Complex & Quantitat Linguist Lab, ES-08034 Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
[4] Univ Michigan, Dept Ecol & Evolutionary Biol, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 USA
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
communication; language; primate; Menzerath; Zipf; ALTMANN LAW; THEROPITHECUS-GELADA; SELF-ORGANIZATION; EVOLUTION; VOCALIZATIONS; COMPLEXITY; LANGUAGE; BREVITY; COST; SIZE;
D O I
10.1073/pnas.1522072113
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Identifying universal principles underpinning diverse natural systems is a key goal of the life sciences. A powerful approach in addressing this goal has been to test whether patterns consistent with linguistic laws are found in nonhuman animals. Menzerath's law is a linguistic law that states that, the larger the construct, the smaller the size of its constituents. Here, to our knowledge, we present the first evidence that Menzerath's law holds in the vocal communication of a nonhuman species. We show that, in vocal sequences of wild male geladas (Theropithecus gelada), construct size (sequence size in number of calls) is negatively correlated with constituent size (duration of calls). Call duration does not vary significantly with position in the sequence, but call sequence composition does change with sequence size and most call types are abbreviated in larger sequences. We also find that intercall intervals follow the same relationship with sequence size as do calls. Finally, we provide formal mathematical support for the idea that Menzerath's law reflects compression-the principle of minimizing the expected length of a code. Our findings suggest that a common principle underpins human and gelada vocal communication, highlighting the value of exploring the applicability of linguistic laws in vocal systems outside the realm of language.
引用
收藏
页码:E2750 / E2758
页数:9
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Moore's Law: A Hard Act to Follow
    DeBenedictis, Erik P.
    [J]. COMPUTER, 2019, 52 (12) : 114 - 117
  • [32] Newton's Law of Cooling: Follow up and exploration
    Maruyama, Shigenao
    Moriya, Shuichi
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HEAT AND MASS TRANSFER, 2021, 164
  • [33] Do dynamical systems follow Benford's law?
    Tolle, CR
    Budzien, JL
    LaViolette, RA
    [J]. CHAOS, 2000, 10 (02) : 331 - 336
  • [34] DO BIOMETRIC IMAGES FOLLOW BENFORD'S LAW?
    Iorliam, Aamo
    Ho, Anthony T. S.
    Poh, Norman
    Shi, Yun Q.
    [J]. 2ND INTERNATIONAL WORKSHOP ON BIOMETRICS AND FORENSICS (IWBF2014), 2014,
  • [35] A LINGUISTIC APPROACH TO LATE QING CHINA'S ENCOUNTER WITH INTERNATIONAL LAW
    Li, Mingqian
    [J]. TSINGHUA CHINA LAW REVIEW, 2022, 14 (02): : 283 - 305
  • [36] An Immigrant's Run-in with the Law: A Forensic Linguistic Analysis.
    Wennerstrom, Ann
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND THE LAW, 2010, 17 (01) : 167 - 170
  • [37] Zipf's Law of Vasovagal Heart Rate Variability Sequences
    Fortrat, Jacques-Olivier
    [J]. ENTROPY, 2020, 22 (04)
  • [38] ON NON-LINEAR RECURSIVE SEQUENCES AND BENFORD'S LAW
    Romano, Paul K.
    McLaughlin, Harry
    [J]. FIBONACCI QUARTERLY, 2011, 49 (02): : 134 - 138
  • [39] Benford’s law and distribution functions of sequences in (0, 1)
    V. Baláž
    K. Nagasaka
    O. Strauch
    [J]. Mathematical Notes, 2010, 88 : 449 - 463
  • [40] CHOVER'S LAW OF THE ITERATED LOGARITHM FOR NEGATIVELY ASSOCIATED SEQUENCES
    Qunying WU·Yuanying JIANG College of Science
    [J]. Journal of Systems Science & Complexity, 2010, 23 (02) : 293 - 302