Top-Down Processes in Simulated Electric-Acoustic Hearing: The Effect of Linguistic Context on Bimodal Benefit for Temporally Interrupted Speech

被引:10
|
作者
Oh, Soo Hee [1 ]
Donaldson, Gail S. [1 ]
Kong, Ying-Yee [2 ]
机构
[1] Univ S Florida, Dept Commun Sci & Disorders, PCD 1017,4202 East Fowler Ave, Tampa, FL 33620 USA
[2] Northeastern Univ, Dept Commun Sci & Disorders, Boston, MA 02115 USA
来源
EAR AND HEARING | 2016年 / 37卷 / 05期
关键词
Bimodal hearing; Cochlear implant; Electric-acoustic stimulation; Linguistic context; COCHLEAR IMPLANT USERS; LOW-FREQUENCY SPEECH; FUNDAMENTAL-FREQUENCY; PHONEMIC RESTORATIONS; RECOGNITION; PERCEPTION; STIMULATION; LISTENERS; SENTENCES; CUES;
D O I
10.1097/AUD.0000000000000298
中图分类号
R36 [病理学]; R76 [耳鼻咽喉科学];
学科分类号
100104 ; 100213 ;
摘要
Objectives: Previous studies have documented the benefits of bimodal hearing as compared with a cochlear implant alone, but most have focused on the importance of bottom-up, low-frequency cues. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the role of top-down processing in bimodal hearing by measuring the effect of sentence context on bimodal benefit for temporally interrupted sentences. It was hypothesized that low-frequency acoustic cues would facilitate the use of contextual information in the interrupted sentences, resulting in greater bimodal benefit for the higher context (CUNY) sentences than for the lower context (IEEE) sentences. Design: Young normal-hearing listeners were tested in simulated bimodal listening conditions in which noise band vocoded sentences were presented to one ear with or without low-pass (LP) filtered speech or LP harmonic complexes (LPHCs) presented to the contralateral ear. Speech recognition scores were measured in three listening conditions: vocoder-alone, vocoder combined with LP speech, and vocoder combined with LPHCs. Temporally interrupted versions of the CUNY and IEEE sentences were used to assess listeners' ability to fill in missing segments of speech by using top-down linguistic processing. Sentences were square-wave gated at a rate of 5 Hz with a 50% duty cycle. Three vocoder channel conditions were tested for each type of sentence (8, 12, and 16 channels for CUNY; 12, 16, and 32 channels for IEEE) and bimodal benefit was compared for similar amounts of spectral degradation (matched-channel comparisons) and similar ranges of baseline performance. Two gain measures, percentage-point gain and normalized gain, were examined. Results: Significant effects of context on bimodal benefit were observed when LP speech was presented to the residual-hearing ear. For the matched-channel comparisons, CUNY sentences showed significantly higher normalized gains than IEEE sentences for both the 12-channel (20 points higher) and 16-channel (18 points higher) conditions. For the individual gain comparisons that used a similar range of baseline performance, CUNY sentences showed bimodal benefits that were significantly higher (7% points, or 15 points normalized gain) than those for IEEE sentences. The bimodal benefits observed here for temporally interrupted speech were considerably smaller than those observed in an earlier study that used continuous speech. Furthermore, unlike previous findings for continuous speech, no bimodal benefit was observed when LPHCs were presented to the LP ear. Conclusions: Findings indicate that linguistic context has a significant influence on bimodal benefit for temporally interrupted speech and support the hypothesis that low-frequency acoustic information presented to the residual-hearing ear facilitates the use of top-down linguistic processing in bimodal hearing. However, bimodal benefit is reduced for temporally interrupted speech as compared with continuous speech, suggesting that listeners' ability to restore missing speech information depends not only on top-down linguistic knowledge but also on the quality of the bottom-up sensory input.
引用
收藏
页码:582 / 592
页数:11
相关论文
共 9 条
  • [1] Low-frequency speech cues and simulated electric-acoustic hearing
    Brown, Christopher A.
    Bacon, Sid P.
    [J]. JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 2009, 125 (03): : 1658 - 1665
  • [2] Effect of Speech Degradation on Top-Down Repair: Phonemic Restoration with Simulations of Cochlear Implants and Combined Electric-Acoustic Stimulation
    Baskent, Deniz
    [J]. JARO-JOURNAL OF THE ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN OTOLARYNGOLOGY, 2012, 13 (05): : 683 - 692
  • [3] The effect of visual cues on top-down restoration of temporally interrupted speech, with and without further degradations
    Benard, Michel R.
    Baskent, Deniz
    [J]. HEARING RESEARCH, 2015, 328 : 24 - 33
  • [4] Interactions Between Slopes of Residual Hearing and Frequency Maps in Simulated Bimodal and Electric-Acoustic Stimulation Hearing
    Yoon, Yang-Soo
    Straw, Shea
    [J]. JOURNAL OF SPEECH LANGUAGE AND HEARING RESEARCH, 2024, 67 (01): : 282 - 295
  • [5] Individual differences in top-down restoration of interrupted speech: Links to linguistic and cognitive abilities
    Benard, Michel Ruben
    Mensink, Jorien Susanne
    Baskent, Deniz
    [J]. JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 2014, 135 (02): : EL88 - EL94
  • [6] Effect of Carrier Bandwidth on Understanding Mandarin Sentences in Simulated Electric-acoustic Hearing
    Wang, Feng
    Chen, Jing
    Chen, Fei
    [J]. INTERSPEECH 2021, 2021, : 611 - 615
  • [7] Effect of F0 contours on top-down repair of interrupted speech
    Clarke, Jeanne
    Kazanoglu, Deniz
    Baskent, Deniz
    Gaudrain, Etienne
    [J]. JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 2017, 142 (01): : EL7 - EL12
  • [8] Effect of Speech Degradation on Top-Down Repair: Phonemic Restoration with Simulations of Cochlear Implants and Combined Electric–Acoustic Stimulation
    Deniz Başkent
    [J]. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 2012, 13 : 683 - 692
  • [9] Does Semantic Context Benefit Speech Understanding through "Top-Down" Processes? Evidence from Time-resolved Sparse fMRI
    Davis, Matthew H.
    Ford, Michael A.
    Kherif, Ferath
    Johnsrude, Ingrid S.
    [J]. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2011, 23 (12) : 3914 - 3932