Paradigm shift in causal thinking in epidemiology

被引:0
|
作者
Shapiro, S. [1 ]
机构
[1] Columbia Univ, Joseph L Mailman Sch Publ Hlth, New York, NY 10032 USA
关键词
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
Q5 [生物化学]; Q7 [分子生物学];
学科分类号
071010 ; 081704 ;
摘要
Professor Shapiro expressed concern with the general direction of epidemiology. Whereas the previous position was that we are unable to interpret limited data, this emphasis has shifted. In particular, the randomized controlled trial (RCT) now is unassailable in the US. Shapiro identified the current issues as being (1) a loss of scepticism in the field, (2) the illusion of accuracy and validity fostered by large databases and their seduction into believing statistical significance, and (3) an over-interpretation of RCTs. Examples of significant errors in epidemiology include spurious associations found for reserpine and breast cancer, calcium channel blocker and cancers in a follow-up study, and fertility drugs and ovarian cancers shown in meta-analysis. The lesson is that one must always be sceptical. The duty of epidemiologists engaged in research is to assume the null hypothesis. They should accept this until the null can be rejected with confidence. Related to the loss of scepticism are the following. Small relative risks are often uninterpretable. If there are fragile data based on small numbers then the data need to be almost free of error or biases. If not, they can change the estimate in such a way as to make the result uninterpretable. Black box statistics might produce nontransparent results. Particularly the meta-analysis of data is often misleading. When causal inference is made in epidemiology, there is a hierarchy of study types as to the level of proof they supply. From highest to lowest, these are RCT, cohorts, case-control, cross-sectional, ecological and descriptive studies. Case-control and cohort studies are simply alternate methods of sampling the population with neither method in principle superior to the other. RCTs are still considered to provide the highest level of proof, but in epidemiology, they cease to be RCTs. RCTs covering a time of 3-7 years become epidemiological follow-up studies, similar to regular cohort approaches. An example of how a study designed to be a randomized trial turns into an observational study is the WHI study, which explored oestrogen plus progestin (EP) vs a placebo. The clinical prediction is that women will experience some specific symptoms during menopause. This is confirmed by the fact that the proportion that unblinded due to the occurrence of vaginal bleeding in the EP group was 44.4% and 6.2% in placebo. The relative risk (RR) of this is 6.5, the risk difference is 37.6%. In addition, there is the potential for detection bias, as women are going to examine their breast more carefully because they are on HRT, if they are unblinded. The cumulative proportion of discontinuation in the RRT group was 42%, in placebo 38%. There is a large pool of women with otherwise silent breast cancer. The annual incidence of breast cancer was 3.8/1,000 in WHI for EP, 3.0/1,000 for placebo, the hazard ratio (HR) was 1.26 (1.00-1.59; p > 0.05). The corresponding risk difference is 0.8/1,000 per year. Other outcomes show similarly small differences, with an excess of 0.07% for CHD, 0.08% for stroke, pulmonary embolism 0.08%, i.e. each with an approximate risk difference of 1/1,000. All these are enormously susceptible to bias, particularly pulmonary embolism. The Million Women Study showed a risk estimate of 1.3 (1.22-1.38) for oestrogen and 2.0 for combined therapy; for tibolone it was 1.44. When seen on the population scale, this amounts to an incidence among EP users of 5.6/1,000 per year; the excess is 2.5/1000 per year. It cannot be said from these data whether HRT does or does not increase the risk because epidemiology is too crude a science.
引用
收藏
页码:127 / 133
页数:7
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [21] Systems thinking for strengthening health systems in LMICs: need for a paradigm shift
    Adam, Taghreed
    de Savigny, Don
    HEALTH POLICY AND PLANNING, 2012, 27 : 1 - 3
  • [22] The changing epidemiology of acute flaccid paralysis warrants a paradigm shift in surveillance
    Messacar, Kevin
    Abzug, Mark J.
    Dominguez, Samuel R.
    JOURNAL OF MEDICAL VIROLOGY, 2018, 90 (01) : 1 - 2
  • [23] Dengue fever in Pakistan: a paradigm shift; changing epidemiology and clinical patterns
    Haider, Zahra
    Ahmad, Farina Zia
    Mahmood, Asif
    Waseem, Tariq
    Shafiq, Irfan
    Raza, Tanzeem
    Qazi, Javaria
    Siddique, Nasir
    Humayun, Malik Asif
    PERSPECTIVES IN PUBLIC HEALTH, 2015, 135 (06) : 294 - 298
  • [24] Assessing causality in epidemiology: revisiting Bradford Hill to incorporate developments in causal thinking
    Shimonovich, Michal
    Pearce, Anna
    Thomson, Hilary
    Keyes, Katherine
    Katikireddi, Srinivasa Vittal
    EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY, 2021, 36 (09) : 873 - 887
  • [25] Assessing causality in epidemiology: revisiting Bradford Hill to incorporate developments in causal thinking
    Michal Shimonovich
    Anna Pearce
    Hilary Thomson
    Katherine Keyes
    Srinivasa Vittal Katikireddi
    European Journal of Epidemiology, 2021, 36 : 873 - 887
  • [26] Charcot neuroarthropathy in persons with diabetes: It's time for a paradigm shift in our thinking
    Wukich, Dane K.
    Frykberg, Robert G.
    Kavarthapu, Venu
    DIABETES-METABOLISM RESEARCH AND REVIEWS, 2024, 40 (03)
  • [27] Moving Toward a Paradigm Shift by Developing that Paradigm Shift
    Martin, Robert J.
    CONSTRUCTIVIST FOUNDATIONS, 2017, 13 (01): : 25 - 27
  • [28] Selection Bias in Clinical Epidemiology Causal Thinking to Guide Patient-centered Research
    Glymour, M. Maria
    Mayeda, Elizabeth Rose
    Selby, Van N.
    EPIDEMIOLOGY, 2016, 27 (04) : 466 - 468
  • [29] PARADIGM EPIDEMIOLOGY
    STREITBERG, B
    BIOMETRICAL JOURNAL, 1992, 34 (04) : 437 - 442
  • [30] Paradigm shift
    Vicente Guillen, Rosario
    ANGIOLOGIA, 2019, 71 (04): : 123 - 126