A Role for Control in an Action-Specific Effect on Perception

被引:8
|
作者
Witt, Jessica K. [1 ]
机构
[1] Colorado State Univ, Ft Collins, CO 80523 USA
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
action-specific perception; speed perception; perception-action relationships; embodied cognition; INFLUENCE PERCEIVED SPEED; VISUAL GUIDANCE; EMBODIED PERCEPTION; DISTANCE PERCEPTION; GEOGRAPHICAL SLANT; SCALED INFORMATION; RESPONSE BIAS; SIZE; AFFORDANCES; EASE;
D O I
10.1037/xhp0000447
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
According to the action-specific account of perception, people perceive the spatial layout of the environment in relation to their ability to act. Pioneering research by Bhalla and Proffitt (1999) demonstrated that hills were judged as steeper to perceivers with less physiological potential. Since this seminal work, much research has shown these action-specific effects generalize beyond hill slant perception and beyond physiological potential, but the underlying mechanisms are underspecified. The present experiments explore the potential mechanism that information about action is integrated with visual information about the target. According to an integration account, information from various sources are weighted, and the strength of these weights dictates the strength of that source of information on the resulting percept. One prediction is that it should be possible to vary the strength of the weights and thereby vary the size of a particular effect. To reduce the effect of action on perception, control over the action was taken away from participants. As predicted, losing control reduced the impact of action on spatial perception. This is the first reported instance of a partial action-specific effect, and is consistent with an integration-based mechanism.
引用
收藏
页码:1791 / 1804
页数:14
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum
    Samejima, K
    Ueda, Y
    Doya, K
    Kimura, M
    SCIENCE, 2005, 310 (5752) : 1337 - 1340
  • [32] The illusion of having a large virtual body biases action-specific perception in patients with mild cognitive impairment
    Ryu, Hokyoung
    Seo, Kyoungwon
    SCIENTIFIC REPORTS, 2021, 11 (01)
  • [33] Recognizing Actions Through Action-Specific Person Detection
    Khan, Fahad Shahbaz
    Xu, Jiaolong
    van de Weijer, Joost
    Bagdanov, Andrew D.
    Anwer, Rao Muhammad
    Lopez, Antonio M.
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON IMAGE PROCESSING, 2015, 24 (11) : 4422 - 4432
  • [34] Proposed applications of research on action-specific effects are premature
    Loomis, Jack M.
    JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION, 2016, 5 (01) : 77 - 79
  • [35] The illusion of having a large virtual body biases action-specific perception in patients with mild cognitive impairment
    Hokyoung Ryu
    Kyoungwon Seo
    Scientific Reports, 11
  • [36] Reinforcement Learning with Action-Specific Focuses in Video Games
    Wang Meng
    Chen Yingfeng
    Lv Tangjie
    Song Yan
    Guan Kai
    Fan Changjie
    Yu Yang
    2020 IEEE CONFERENCE ON GAMES (IEEE COG 2020), 2020, : 9 - 16
  • [37] Action-specific activity in response to observed tool use actions
    Frey, S
    Gerry, V
    Grafton, S
    JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2005, : 248 - 248
  • [38] Supporting human operators by integrating action-specific warnings and instructions
    Schmuntzsch, U.
    Reichmuth, R.
    Roetting, M.
    AUTOMATION 2012, 2012, 2171 : 411 - 414
  • [39] Crossmodal and action-specific: neuroimaging the human mirror neuron system
    Oosterhof, Nikolaas N.
    Tipper, Steven P.
    Downing, Paul E.
    TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2013, 17 (07) : 311 - 318
  • [40] What you see and what you are told: an action-specific effect that is unaffected by explicit feedback
    Zachary R. King
    Nathan L. Tenhundfeld
    Jessica K. Witt
    Psychological Research, 2018, 82 : 507 - 519