A Time-Dependent Saliency Model Combining Center and Depth Biases for 2D and 3D Viewing Conditions

被引:17
|
作者
Gautier, J. [1 ]
Le Meur, O. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Rennes 1, F-35042 Rennes, France
关键词
Eye movements; Saliency model; Binocular disparity; Stereoscopy; VISUAL-ATTENTION; EYE-MOVEMENTS; SCENE; FEATURES;
D O I
10.1007/s12559-012-9138-3
中图分类号
TP18 [人工智能理论];
学科分类号
081104 ; 0812 ; 0835 ; 1405 ;
摘要
The role of the binocular disparity in the deployment of visual attention is examined in this paper. To address this point, we compared eye tracking data recorded while observers viewed natural images in 2D and 3D conditions. The influence of disparity on saliency, center and depth biases is first studied. Results show that visual exploration is affected by the introduction of the binocular disparity. In particular, participants tend to look first at closer areas in 3D condition and then direct their gaze to more widespread locations. Beside this behavioral analysis, we assess the extent to which state-of-the-art models of bottom-up visual attention predict where observers looked at in both viewing conditions. To improve their ability to predict salient regions, low-level features as well as higher-level foreground/background cues are examined. Results indicate that, consecutively to initial centering response, the foreground feature plays an active role in the early but also middle instants of attention deployments. Importantly, this influence is more pronounced in stereoscopic conditions. It supports the notion of a quasi-instantaneous bottom-up saliency modulated by higher figure/ground processing. Beyond depth information itself, the foreground cue might constitute an early process of "selection for action". Finally, we propose a time-dependent computational model to predict saliency on still pictures. The proposed approach combines low-level visual features, center and depth biases. Its performance outperforms state-of-the-art models of bottom-up attention.
引用
收藏
页码:141 / 156
页数:16
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Numerosity and density estimation: biases for 2D area but not for 3D volume
    Bell, J.
    Manson, A.
    Edwards, M.
    PERCEPTION, 2014, 43 (01) : 49 - 49
  • [32] 2D/3D convertible display with enhanced 3D viewing region based on integral imaging
    Park, Soon-gi
    Song, Byoung-Sub
    Min, Sung-Wook
    STEREOSCOPIC DISPLAYS AND APPLICATIONS XXI, 2010, 7524
  • [33] Membrane mirror based display for viewing 2D and 3D images
    McKay, S
    Mason, S
    Mair, LS
    Waddell, P
    Fraser, SM
    PROJECTION DISPLAYS V, 1999, 3634 : 144 - 155
  • [34] Fast pressure calculation for 2D and 3D time dependent incompressible flow
    van Kan, J
    Vuik, C
    Wesseling, P
    NUMERICAL LINEAR ALGEBRA WITH APPLICATIONS, 2000, 7 (06) : 429 - 447
  • [35] Design in 2D, model in 3D: Live 3D pose generation from 2D sketches
    Tosco, Paolo
    Mackey, Mark
    Cheeseright, Tim
    ABSTRACTS OF PAPERS OF THE AMERICAN CHEMICAL SOCIETY, 2019, 258
  • [36] Combining 3D and 2D for less constrained periocular recognition
    Chen, Lulu
    Ferryman, James
    2015 IEEE 7TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON BIOMETRICS THEORY, APPLICATIONS AND SYSTEMS (BTAS 2015), 2015,
  • [37] Numerical simulation of the time-dependent deformation behaviour of claystone rock mass at the Tournemire site with 2D and 3D models
    Rutenberg, Michael
    Lux, Karl-Heinz
    PHYSICS AND CHEMISTRY OF THE EARTH, 2011, 36 (17-18) : 1913 - 1921
  • [38] The effect of finite depth on 2D and 3D cavitating hydrofoils
    Bal, Sakir
    JOURNAL OF MARINE SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY, 2011, 16 (02) : 129 - 142
  • [39] The effect of finite depth on 2D and 3D cavitating hydrofoils
    Sakir Bal
    Journal of Marine Science and Technology, 2011, 16 : 129 - 142
  • [40] Depth processing in 2D representations of 3D objects: Is there any?
    Ray, RH
    AUSTRALIAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 54 (01) : 51 - 52