Late song learning in song sparrows

被引:41
|
作者
Nordby, JC
Campbell, SE
Beecher, MD
机构
[1] Univ Washington, Dept Psychol, Anim Behav Program, Seattle, WA 98195 USA
[2] Univ Washington, Dept Zool, Seattle, WA 98195 USA
基金
美国国家卫生研究院; 美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
D O I
10.1006/anbe.2000.1673
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
In many oscine species, young males learn songs that match those of their first breeding-season neighbours. Because sharing songs with neighbours may be advantageous, selection should favour birds that retain the capacity to memorize new songs later in their first year as the birds cannot know for sure who their neighbours will be until spring. We investigated whether song sparrows, Melospiza melodia, from a sedentary western population were capable of acquiring new songs after their natal summer in a two-stage laboratory experiment. In the first stage (30-90 days of age), we rotated hand-roared males equally among one set of four live tutors that had been neighbours in the field (and therefore shared songs between them). During the second stage (140-330 days old), we removed two of the original tutors and replaced them with two new tutors (which did not share any songs with the original tutors). During stage two, subjects were not rotated, but were stationed next to only one of the four tutors (they could hear the other three at a distance). Eight of 12 subjects learned songs from tutors they only heard after they were 140 days old, and six subjects learned most of their songs from a late tutor. Thus, sedentary song sparrows are capable of acquiring many; songs de novo in late autumn. These results are consistent with a song-learning strategy that provides young male song sparrows with a repertoire of songs they will share with their first breeding-season neighbours. (C) 2001 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.
引用
收藏
页码:835 / 846
页数:12
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] SPECIES SONG DISCRIMINATION IN ADULT FEMALE SONG AND SWAMP SPARROWS
    SEARCY, WA
    MARLER, P
    PETERS, SS
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1981, 29 (NOV) : 997 - 1003
  • [42] VARIATION IN SONGS OF SONG SPARROWS
    HARRIS, MA
    AMERICAN ZOOLOGIST, 1975, 15 (03): : 804 - 804
  • [43] The response of male and female song sparrows to geographic variation in song
    Searcy, WA
    Nowicki, S
    Hughes, M
    CONDOR, 1997, 99 (03): : 651 - 657
  • [44] Sexual preferences for mate song types in female song sparrows
    OLoghlen, AL
    Beecher, MD
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1997, 53 : 835 - 841
  • [45] SONG SPARROWS HAVE TOP 10 SONG PREFERENCE LIST
    MULLIGAN, JA
    AMERICAN ZOOLOGIST, 1980, 20 (04): : 725 - 725
  • [46] Soft song is a reliable signal of aggressive intent in song sparrows
    Templeton, Christopher N.
    Akcay, Caglar
    Campbell, S. Elizabeth
    Beecher, Michael D.
    BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY AND SOCIOBIOLOGY, 2012, 66 (11) : 1503 - 1509
  • [47] Song sharing in two populations of song sparrows (Melospiza melodia)
    Hill, CE
    Campbell, SE
    Nordby, JC
    Burt, JM
    Beecher, MD
    BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY AND SOCIOBIOLOGY, 1999, 46 (05) : 341 - 349
  • [48] Geographic song discrimination in relation to dispersal distances in song sparrows
    Searcy, WA
    Nowicki, S
    Hughes, M
    Peters, S
    AMERICAN NATURALIST, 2002, 159 (03): : 221 - 230
  • [49] Song learning by Chipping Sparrows: When, where, and from whom
    Liu, Wan-Chun
    Kroodsma, Donald E.
    CONDOR, 2006, 108 (03): : 509 - 517
  • [50] Birdsong learning is mutually beneficial for tutee and tutor in song sparrows
    Beecher, Michael D.
    Akcay, Caglar
    Campbell, S. Elizabeth
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2020, 166 : 281 - 288