Individual differences in first- and second-order temporal judgment

被引:5
|
作者
Corcoran, Andrew W. [1 ,2 ]
Groot, Christopher [1 ]
Bruno, Aurelio [3 ]
Johnston, Alan [4 ]
Cropper, Simon J. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Melbourne, Melbourne Sch Psychol Sci, Melbourne, Vic, Australia
[2] Monash Univ, Sch Philosoph Hist & Int Studies, Melbourne, Vic, Australia
[3] UCL, Dept Expt Psychol, London, England
[4] Univ Nottingham, Sch Psychol, Nottingham, England
来源
PLOS ONE | 2018年 / 13卷 / 02期
关键词
TIME PERCEPTION NETWORKS; LATENT PROFILE ANALYSIS; SCHIZOTYPAL PERSONALITY; HEALTHY SCHIZOTYPY; VISUAL CONFIDENCE; SUBJECTIVE TIME; SCHIZOPHRENIA; COGNITION; NEUROCOGNITION; SCALES;
D O I
10.1371/journal.pone.0191422
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
The ability of subjects to identify and reproduce brief temporal intervals is influenced by many factors whether they be stimulus-based, task-based or subject-based. The current study examines the role individual differences play in subsecond and suprasecond timing judgments, using the schizoptypy personality scale as a test-case approach for quantifying a broad range of individual differences. In two experiments, 129 (Experiment 1) and 141 (Experiment 2) subjects completed the O-LIFE personality questionnaire prior to performing a modified temporal-bisection task. In the bisection task, subjects responded to two identical instantiations of a luminance grating presented in a 4deg window, 4deg above fixation for 1.5 s (Experiment 1) or 3 s (Experiment 2). Subjects initiated presentation with a button-press, and released the button when they considered the stimulus to be half-way through (750/1500 ms). Subjects were then asked to indicate their 'most accurate estimate' of the two intervals. In this way we measure both performance on the task (a first-order measure) and the subjects' knowledge of their performance (a second-order measure). In Experiment 1 the effect of grating-drift and feedback on performance was also examined. Experiment 2 focused on the static/no-feedback condition. For the group data, Experiment 1 showed a significant effect of presentation order in the baseline condition (no feedback), which disappeared when feedback was provided. Moving the stimulus had no effect on perceived duration. Experiment 2 showed no effect of stimulus presentation order. This elimination of the subsecond order-effect was at the expense of accuracy, as the mid-point of the suprasecond interval was generally underestimated. Response precision increased as a proportion of total duration, reducing the variance below that predicted by Weber's law. This result is consistent with a breakdown of the scalar properties of time perception in the early suprasecond range. All subjects showed good insight into their own performance, though that insight did not necessarily correlate with the veridical bisection point. In terms of personality, we found evidence of significant differences in performance along the Unusual Experiences subscale, of most theoretical interest here, in the subsecond condition only. There was also significant correlation with Impulsive Nonconformity and Cognitive Disorganisation in the sub-and suprasecond conditions, respectively. Overall, these data support a partial dissociation of timing mechanisms at very short and slightly longer intervals. Further, these results suggest that perception is not the only critical mitigator of confidence in temporal experience, since individuals can effectively compensate for differences in perception at the level of metacognition in early suprasecond time. Though there are individual differences in performance, these are perhaps less than expected from previous reports and indicate an effective timing mechanism dealing with brief durations independent of the influence of significant personality trait differences.
引用
收藏
页数:27
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] A model for knickpoint migration in first- and second-order streams
    Bressan, Filippo
    Papanicolaou, A. N.
    Abban, Benjamin
    GEOPHYSICAL RESEARCH LETTERS, 2014, 41 (14) : 4987 - 4996
  • [42] FIRST- AND SECOND-ORDER DIPOLE MOMENTS OF HOMOPOLAR CRYSTALS
    KEATING, PN
    PHYSICAL REVIEW, 1965, 140 (1A): : A369 - &
  • [43] Inverted First- and Second-Order Elections The Israeli Case
    Nachmias, David
    Rosenthal, Maoz
    Zubida, Hani
    ISRAEL STUDIES REVIEW, 2012, 27 (01) : 31 - 54
  • [44] Area summation of first- and second-order modulations of luminance
    Summers, Robert J.
    Baker, Daniel H.
    Meese, Tim S.
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2015, 15 (01):
  • [45] Forming first- and second-order cardioids with multimode hydrophones
    McConnell, James A.
    Jensen, Scott C.
    OCEANS 2006, VOLS 1-4, 2006, : 65 - +
  • [46] Crowding between first- and second-order letters in amblyopia
    Chung, Susana T. L.
    Li, Roger W.
    Levi, Dennis M.
    VISION RESEARCH, 2008, 48 (06) : 788 - 798
  • [47] First- and second-order Raman spectra of galena (PbS)
    Smith, GD
    Firth, S
    Clark, RJH
    Cardona, M
    JOURNAL OF APPLIED PHYSICS, 2002, 92 (08) : 4375 - 4380
  • [48] Double dissociation between first- and second-order processing
    Allard, Remy
    Faubert, Jocelyn
    VISION RESEARCH, 2007, 47 (09) : 1129 - 1141
  • [49] Emittance manipulation by first- and second-order lattice control
    Lund Univ, Lund, Sweden
    Nucl Instrum Methods Phys Res Sect A, 3 (463-470):
  • [50] Epsilon substitution for first- and second-order predicate logic
    Mints, Grigori
    ANNALS OF PURE AND APPLIED LOGIC, 2013, 164 (06) : 733 - 739