Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging

被引:209
|
作者
Boronat, CB
Buxbaum, LJ
Coslett, HB
Tang, K
Saffran, EM
Kimberg, DY
Detre, JA
机构
[1] Moss Rehabil Res Inst, Philadelphia, PA 19141 USA
[2] Thomas Jefferson Univ, Philadelphia, PA 19107 USA
[3] Univ Penn, Philadelphia, PA 19104 USA
[4] Temple Univ, Philadelphia, PA 19122 USA
来源
COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH | 2005年 / 23卷 / 2-3期
关键词
tools; artifacts; animals; semantic knowledge; parietal lobe; temporal lobe; frontal lobe; apraxia; praxis;
D O I
10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.11.001
中图分类号
TP18 [人工智能理论];
学科分类号
081104 ; 0812 ; 0835 ; 1405 ;
摘要
A prominent account of conceptual knowledge proposes that information is distributed over visual, tactile, auditory, motor and verbal-declarative attribute domains to the degree to which these features were activated when the knowledge was acquired [D.A. Allport, Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphagia, In: S.K. Newman, R. Epstein (Eds.), Current perspectives in dysphagia, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1985, pp. 32-60]. A corollary is that when drawing upon this knowledge (e.g., to answer questions), particular aspects of this distributed information is re-activated as a function of the requirements of the task at hand [L.J. Buxbaum, E.M. Saffran, Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and non-apraxic subjects. Brain and Language, 82 (2002) 179-199; L.J. Buxbaum, T. Veramonti, M.F. Schwartz, Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: knowing `what for' but not 'how', Neurocase, 6 (2000) 83-97; W. Simmons, L. Barsalou, The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20 (2003) 451-186]. This account predicts that answering questions about object manipulation should activate brain regions previously identified as components of the distributed sensory-motor system involved in object use, whereas answering questions about object function (that is, the purpose that it serves) should activate regions identified as components of the systems supporting verbal-declarative features. These predictions were tested in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study in which 15 participants viewed picture or word pairs denoting manipulable objects and determined whether the objects are manipulated similarly (M condition) or serve the same function (F condition). Significantly greater and more extensive activations in the left inferior parietal lobe bordering the intraparietal sulcus were seen in the M condition with pictures and, to a lesser degree, words. These findings are consistent with the known role of this region in skilled object use [K.M. Heilman, L.J. Gonzalez Rothi, Apraxia, In: K.M. Heilman, E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology, Oxford University Press, New York, 1993, pp. 141-150] as well as previous fMRI results [M. Kellenbach, M. Brett, K. Patterson, Actions speak louder than functions: the importance of manipulability and action in tool representation, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15 (2003) 30-46] and behavioral findings in brain-lesion patients [L.J. Buxbaum, E.M. Saffran, Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and non-apraxic subjects, Brain and Language, 82 (2002) 179-199]. No brain regions were significantly more activated in the F than M condition. These data suggest that brain regions specialized for sensory-motor function are a critical component of distributed representations of manipulable objects. (c) 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:361 / 373
页数:13
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Functional magnetic resonance imaging
    Kahan, Joshua
    Auger, Stephen
    BRITISH JOURNAL OF HOSPITAL MEDICINE, 2015, 76 (12) : C189 - C192
  • [32] Functional magnetic resonance imaging
    Matthews, PM
    Jezzard, P
    JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY NEUROSURGERY AND PSYCHIATRY, 2004, 75 (01): : 6 - 12
  • [33] Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
    Shah, Lubdha M.
    Anderson, Jeffrey S.
    Lee, James N.
    Wiggins, Richard, III
    SEMINARS IN ROENTGENOLOGY, 2010, 45 (02) : 147 - 156
  • [34] Function and manipulation knowledge in gesture: Evidence from case studies in aphasia
    Power, E
    Sheard, C
    Code, C
    AUSTRALIAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 53 (02) : 125 - 125
  • [35] Functional anatomy of object recognition in humans: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging
    Bly, BM
    Kosslyn, SM
    CURRENT OPINION IN NEUROLOGY, 1997, 10 (01) : 5 - 9
  • [36] Activation of Manipulation and Function Knowledge During Visual Search for Objects
    Ruotolo, Francesco
    Kalenine, Solene
    Bartolo, Angela
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE, 2020, 46 (01) : 66 - 90
  • [37] Evidence of timing effects on acupuncture: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study
    Gao, Yanling
    Lin, Zhicheng
    Tao, Jing
    Yang, Shanli
    Chen, Ri
    Jiang, Cai
    Dong, Keng
    Huang, Jia
    Chen, Lidian
    EXPERIMENTAL AND THERAPEUTIC MEDICINE, 2015, 9 (01) : 59 - 64
  • [38] Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia
    Gracely, RH
    Petzke, F
    Wolf, JM
    Clauw, DJ
    ARTHRITIS AND RHEUMATISM, 2002, 46 (05): : 1333 - 1343
  • [39] Functional magnetic resonance imaging and multiple sclerosis: The evidence for neuronal plasticity
    Buckle, GJ
    JOURNAL OF NEUROIMAGING, 2005, 15 (04) : 82S - 93S
  • [40] Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex
    Badre, David
    D'Esposito, Mark
    JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2007, 19 (12) : 2082 - 2099