An ERP-study of brand and no-name products

被引:15
|
作者
Thomas, Anika [1 ,2 ]
Hammer, Anke [3 ,4 ]
Beibst, Gabriele [2 ]
Muente, Thomas F. [3 ]
机构
[1] Univ Magdeburg, Dept Neuropsychol, D-39106 Magdeburg, Germany
[2] Univ Appl Sci Jena, Dept Business Adm, Jena, Germany
[3] Med Univ Lubeck, Dept Neurol, D-23538 Lubeck, Germany
[4] Univ Erlangen Nurnberg, Dept Psychiat, D-91054 Erlangen, Germany
来源
BMC NEUROSCIENCE | 2013年 / 14卷
关键词
Go/Nogo; Event-related potentials; Brands; Neuromarketing; Implicit associations; Late positive component; Lateralized readiness potential; EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS; TIME-COURSE; BRAIN POTENTIALS; INDIVIDUAL-DIFFERENCES; PREFRONTAL CORTEX; IMPLICIT MEASURES; HAND MOVEMENT; INFORMATION; GO; ACTIVATION;
D O I
10.1186/1471-2202-14-149
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
Background: Brands create product personalities that are thought to affect consumer decisions. Here we assessed, using the Go/No-go Association Task (GNAT) from social psychology, whether brands as opposed to no-name products are associated with implicit positive attitudes. Healthy young German participants viewed series of photos of cosmetics and food items (half of them brands) intermixed with positive and negative words. In any given run, one category of goods (e.g., cosmetics) and one kind of words (e.g., positive) had to be responded to, whereas responses had to be withheld for the other categories. Event-related brain potentials were recorded during the task. Results: Unexpectedly, there were no response-time differences between congruent (brand and positive words) and incongruent (brand and negative words) pairings but ERPs showed differences as a function of congruency in the 600-750 ms time-window hinting at the existence of implicit attitudes towards brand and no-name stimuli. This finding deserves further investigation in future studies. Moreover, the amplitude of the late positive component (LPC) was found to be enhanced for brand as opposed to no-name stimuli. Conclusions: Congruency effects suggest that ERPs are sensitive to implicit attitudes. Moreover, the results for the LPC imply that pictures of brand products are more arousing than those of no-name products, which may ultimately contribute to consumer decisions.
引用
收藏
页数:9
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Equivalence of generic and brand-name ophthalmic products
    Fiscella, RG
    Gaynes, BI
    Jensen, M
    [J]. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF HEALTH-SYSTEM PHARMACY, 2001, 58 (07) : 616 - 617
  • [33] Processing of food stimuli in anorexia nervosa: An ERP-study comparing adolescents and adults
    Stonawski, Valeska
    Mai-Lippold, Sandra A.
    Graap, Holmer
    Moll, Gunther H.
    Kratz, Oliver
    Van Doren, Jessica
    Horndasch, Stefanie
    [J]. EUROPEAN EATING DISORDERS REVIEW, 2024, 32 (02) : 281 - 297
  • [34] POTENCY OF BRAND NAME AND GENERIC L-THYROXINE PRODUCTS
    STOFFER, SS
    SZPUNAR, WE
    [J]. CLINICAL CHEMISTRY, 1981, 27 (06) : 1070 - 1070
  • [35] Word processing deficits in children with isolated and combined reading and spelling deficits: An ERP-study
    Mehlhase, Heike
    Bakos, Sarolta
    Bartling, Juergen
    Schulte-Koerne, Gerd
    Moll, Kristina
    [J]. BRAIN RESEARCH, 2020, 1738
  • [36] The brand name
    Guimaraes, Eduardo
    [J]. ASTERION-PHILOSOPHIE HISTOIRE DES IDEES PENSEE POLITIQUE, 2011, (08):
  • [37] The effects of brand familiarity and product category in brand extension: An ERP study
    Ma, Qingguo
    Wang, Manlin
    Da, Qian
    [J]. NEUROSCIENCE RESEARCH, 2021, 169 : 48 - 56
  • [38] The Gye, the No-Name Hair Salon, the Coup d'Etat, and the Small Dreamers
    Chae, Jung Hae
    [J]. NEW ENGLAND REVIEW-MIDDLEBURY SERIES, 2021, 42 (04): : 135 - 145
  • [40] Investigation of Bioequivalence Between Brand-name and Generic Irinotecan Products
    Saito, Ken-Ichi
    Inoue, Yutaka
    Ikegami, Yoji
    Nanbo, Izumi
    Onozuka, Mari
    Sano, Kazumi
    Yoshida, Hisahiro
    Sakamoto, Toshihiro
    Tatebayashi, Emi
    Fujita, Ken-Ichi
    Sasaki, Yasutsuna
    Kitazawa, Takaki
    [J]. ANTICANCER RESEARCH, 2016, 36 (11) : 5957 - 5963