The hands' default location guides tactile spatial selectivity

被引:9
|
作者
Badde, Stephanie [1 ]
Heed, Tobias [2 ,3 ]
机构
[1] Tufts Univ, Dept Psychol, Medford, MA 02155 USA
[2] Univ Salzburg, Dept Psychol, Cognit Psychol, A-5020 Salzburg, Austria
[3] Univ Salzburg, Ctr Cognit Neurosci, A-5020 Salzburg, Austria
关键词
tactile; spatial selectivity; body posture; tactile motion; temporal perception; ADAPTATION AFTEREFFECTS REVEAL; APPARENT-MOTION; TEMPORAL-ORDER; TOUCH; INTEGRATION; PERCEPTION; PROPRIOCEPTION; SACCADES; FORM; AREA;
D O I
10.1073/pnas.2209680120
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Our skin is a two-dimensional sheet that can be folded into a multitude of configura-tions due to the mobility of our body parts. Parts of the human tactile system might account for this flexibility by being tuned to locations in the world rather than on the skin. Using adaptation, we scrutinized the spatial selectivity of two tactile perceptual mechanisms for which the visual equivalents have been reported to be selective in world coordinates: tactile motion and the duration of tactile events. Participants' hand posi-tion-uncrossed or crossed-as well as the stimulated hand varied independently across adaptation and test phases. This design distinguished among somatotopic selectivity for locations on the skin and spatiotopic selectivity for locations in the environment, but also tested spatial selectivity that fits neither of these classical reference frames and is based on the default position of the hands. For both features, adaptation consistently affected subsequent tactile perception at the adapted hand, reflecting skin -bound spatial selectivity. Yet, tactile motion and temporal adaptation also transferred across hands but only if the hands were crossed during the adaptation phase, that is, when one hand was placed at the other hand's typical location. Thus, selectivity for locations in the world was based on default rather than online sensory information about the location of the hands. These results challenge the prevalent dichotomy of somatotopic and spatiotopic selectivity and suggest that prior information about the hands' default position -right hand at the right side-is embedded deep in the tactile sensory system.SignificanceWhen we touch objects in the dark, we must take body posture into account to determine the objects' location. This could be achieved by mechanisms that are selective for tactile stimuli located at specific locations in the world irrespective of their location on the skin. Using adaptation, an established behavioral paradigm to probe the properties of neural mechanisms, we indeed found evidence for spatial selectivity beyond skin locations. Yet, this selectivity reflected the hands' typical not their actual location in space. Our result challenges the widely held idea that sensory events are coded exclusively in topographic reference frames and suggests that prior information about the location of the hands is embedded in the tactile sensory system.
引用
收藏
页数:6
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [21] Crossing the hands disrupts tactile spatial attention but not motor attention: Evidence from event-related potentials
    Gherri, Elena
    Forster, Bettina
    NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2012, 50 (09) : 2303 - 2316
  • [22] A Conceptual Model of Tactile Processing across Body Features of Size, Shape, Side, and Spatial Location
    Tame, Luigi
    Azanon, Elena
    Longo, Matthew R.
    FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2019, 10
  • [23] MODIFYING AN UNDERLYING COMPONENT OF PERCEIVED ARM LENGTH - ADAPTATION OF TACTILE LOCATION INDUCED BY SPATIAL DISCORDANCE
    CRASKE, B
    KENNY, FT
    KEITH, D
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-HUMAN PERCEPTION AND PERFORMANCE, 1984, 10 (02) : 307 - 317
  • [24] On default priors and approximate location models
    Fraser, D. A. S.
    Reid, N.
    BRAZILIAN JOURNAL OF PROBABILITY AND STATISTICS, 2011, 25 (03) : 353 - 361
  • [25] A four-channel analysis of the tactile sensitivity of the fingertip: frequency selectivity, spatial summation, and temporal summation
    Gescheider, GA
    Bolanowski, SJ
    Pope, JV
    Verrillo, RT
    SOMATOSENSORY AND MOTOR RESEARCH, 2002, 19 (02): : 114 - 124
  • [26] Object-Guided Spatial Selection in Touch Without Concurrent Changes in the Perceived Location of the Hands
    Gillmeister, Helge
    Cantarella, Simona
    Gheorghiu, Ana Ioana
    Adler, Julia
    EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2013, 60 (01) : 64 - 70
  • [27] Location-specific citizenship: state visions of spatial selectivity in the cities of Beijing and Suzhou
    Cheung, Paul
    TERRITORY POLITICS GOVERNANCE, 2024, 12 (02) : 278 - 296
  • [28] An integrated tactile feedback system for multifingered robot hands
    Lo, WT
    Shen, YT
    Liu, YH
    IROS 2001: PROCEEDINGS OF THE 2001 IEEE/RJS INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON INTELLIGENT ROBOTS AND SYSTEMS, VOLS 1-4: EXPANDING THE SOCIETAL ROLE OF ROBOTICS IN THE NEXT MILLENNIUM, 2001, : 680 - 685
  • [29] Safely Grasping with Complex Dexterous Hands by Tactile Feedback
    Sanchez, Jose
    Schneider, Sven
    Ploeger, Paul
    ROBOCUP 2014: ROBOT WORLD CUP XVIII, 2015, 8992 : 332 - 344
  • [30] Distributed Tactile Sensors for Palmar Surfaces of Prosthetic Hands
    Hoang Truong
    Correll, Nikolaus
    Segil, Jacob
    2023 11TH INTERNATIONAL IEEE/EMBS CONFERENCE ON NEURAL ENGINEERING, NER, 2023,