Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning

被引:0
|
作者
Raju S. Bapi
Kenji Doya
Alexander M. Harner
机构
[1] Computational Neurobiology Group,
[2] Kawato Dynamic Brain Project,undefined
[3] ERATO,undefined
[4] JST,undefined
[5] 2–2 Hikaridai,undefined
[6] Seika,undefined
[7] Soraku,undefined
[8] Kyoto 619–0288,undefined
[9] Japan,undefined
[10] Department of Psychology,undefined
[11] Boston University,undefined
[12] Boston,undefined
[13] MA 02215,undefined
[14] USA,undefined
[15] Present address: Department of Computer and Information Sciences,undefined
[16] University of Hyderabad,undefined
[17] Gachibowli,undefined
[18] Hyderabad 500 046,undefined
[19] India,undefined
[20] e-mail: rajubapi@hotmail.com,undefined
[21] Tel.: +91-40-3010780,undefined
[22] Fax: +91-40-3010120/3010145,undefined
来源
关键词
Sequence representation Effector dependence Effector independence Skill learning Transfer of sequence knowledge Time course of sequence learning;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
To investigate the representation of motor sequence, we tested transfer effects in a motor sequence learning paradigm. We hypothesize that there are two sequence representations, effector independent and dependent. Further, we postulate that the effector independent representation is in visual/spatial coordinates, that the effector dependent representation is in motor coordinates, and that their time courses of acquisition during learning are different. Twelve subjects were tested in a modified 2×10 task. Subjects learned to press two keys (called a set) successively on a keypad in response to two lighted squares on a 3×3 display. The complete sequence to be learned was composed of ten such sets, called a hyperset. Training was given in the normal condition and sequence recall was assessed in the early, intermediate, and late stages in three conditions, normal, visual, and motor. In the visual condition, finger–keypad mapping was rotated 90° while the keypad–display mapping was kept identical to normal. In the motor condition, the keypad–display mapping was also rotated 90°, resulting in an identical finger–display mapping as in normal. Subjects formed two groups with each group using a different normal condition. One group learned the sequence in a standard keypad–hand setting and subsequently recalled the sequence using a rotated keypad–hand setting in the test conditions. The second group learned the sequence with a rotated keypad–hand setting and subsequently recalled the sequence with a standard keypad–hand setting in the test conditions. Response time (RT) and sequencing errors during recall were recorded. Although subjects committed more sequencing errors in both testing conditions, visual and motor, as compared to the normal condition, the errors were below chance level. Sequencing errors did not differ significantly between visual and motor conditions. Further, the sequence recall accuracy was over 70% even by the early stage when the subjects performed the sequence for the first time with the altered conditions, visual and motor. There were parallel improvements thereafter in all the conditions. These results of positive transfer of sequence knowledge across conditions that use dissimilar finger movements point to an effector independent sequence representation, possibly in visual/spatial coordinates. Initially the RTs were similar in the visual and the motor conditions, but with training RTs in the motor condition became significantly shorter than in the visual condition, as revealed by significant interaction for the testing stage and condition term in the repeated measures ANOVA. Moreover, using RTs for single key pressing in the three conditions as baseline indices, it was again observed that RTs in the visual and motor conditions were not significantly different in the early stage, but motor RTs became significantly shorter by the late testing stage. These results support the hypothesis that the motor condition benefits more than the visual because it uses identical effector movements to the normal condition. Further, these results argue for the existence of effector dependent sequence representation, in motor coordinates, which is acquired relatively slowly. The difference in the time course of learning of these two representations may account for the differential involvement of brain areas in early and late learning phases found in lesion and imaging studies.
引用
下载
收藏
页码:149 / 162
页数:13
相关论文
共 40 条
  • [1] Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning
    Bapi, RS
    Doya, K
    Harner, AM
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2000, 132 (02) : 149 - 162
  • [2] Effector-Independent and Effector-Dependent Sequence Representations Underlie General and Specific Perceptuomotor Sequence Learning
    Andresen, David R.
    Marsolek, Chad J.
    JOURNAL OF MOTOR BEHAVIOR, 2012, 44 (01) : 53 - 61
  • [3] Bihemispheric Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Effector-Independent Representations of Motor Synergy and Sequence Learning
    Waters-Metenier, Sheena
    Husain, Masud
    Wiestler, Tobias
    Diedrichsen, Joern
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2014, 34 (03): : 1037 - 1050
  • [4] Effector-Independent Motor Sequence Representations Exist in Extrinsic and Intrinsic Reference Frames
    Wiestler, Tobias
    Waters-Metenier, Sheena
    Diedrichsen, Joern
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2014, 34 (14): : 5054 - 5064
  • [5] Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task
    Verwey, WB
    Wright, DL
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2004, 68 (01): : 64 - 70
  • [6] Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task
    Willem B. Verwey
    David L. Wright
    Psychological Research, 2004, 68 : 64 - 70
  • [7] Motor resonance in left- and right-handers: evidence for effector-independent motor representations
    Sartori, Luisa
    Begliomini, Chiara
    Castiello, Umberto
    FRONTIERS IN HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE, 2013, 7
  • [8] Time course of learning sequence representations in action imagery practice
    Dahm, Stephan F.
    Rieger, Martina
    HUMAN MOVEMENT SCIENCE, 2023, 87
  • [9] The time course of off-line motor sequence learning
    Press, DZ
    Casement, MD
    Pascual-Leone, A
    Robertson, EM
    COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH, 2005, 25 (01): : 375 - 378
  • [10] The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study
    Toni, I
    Krams, M
    Turner, R
    Passingham, RE
    NEUROIMAGE, 1998, 8 (01) : 50 - 61