A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence

被引:264
|
作者
Imai, M [1 ]
Gentner, D [1 ]
机构
[1] NORTHWESTERN UNIV,DEPT PSYCHOL,EVANSTON,IL 60201
基金
美国国家科学基金会;
关键词
D O I
10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00784-6
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
This research concerns how children learn the distinction between substance names and object names. Quine (1969) proposed that children learn the distinction through learning the syntactic distinctions inherent in count/mass grammar. However, So-a et al. (1991) found that English-speaking 2-year-olds, who did not seem to have acquired count/mass grammar, distinguished objects from substances in a word extension task, suggesting a pre-linguistic ontological distinction. To test whether the distinction between object names and substance names is conceptually or linguistically driven, we repeated Soja et al.'s study with English- and Japanese-speaking 2-, 2.5.-, and 4-year-olds and adults. Japanese does not make a count-mass grammatical distinction: all inanimate nouns are treated alike. Thus if young Japanese children made the object-substance distinction in word meaning, this would support the early ontology position over the linguistic influence position. We used three types of standards: substances (e.g., sand in an S-shape), simple objects (e.g., a kidney-shaped piece of paraffin) and complex objects (e.g., a wood whisk). The subjects learned novel nouns in neutral syntax denoting each standard entity. They were then asked which of the two alternatives - one matching in shape but not material and the other matching in material but not shape - would also be named by the same label. The results suggest the universal use of ontological knowledge in early word learning. Children in both languages showed differentiation between (complex) objects and substances as early as 2 years of age. However, there were also early cross-linguistic differences. American and Japanese children generalized the simple object instances and the substance instances differently. We speculate that children universally make a distinction between individuals and non-individuals in word learning but that the nature of the categories and the boundary between them is influenced by language. (C) 1997 Elsevier Science B.V.
引用
收藏
页码:169 / 200
页数:32
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] Word: A cross-linguistic typology
    Vajda, Edward J.
    [J]. WORD-JOURNAL OF THE INTERNATIONAL LINGUISTIC ASSOCIATION, 2006, 57 (01): : 154 - 157
  • [2] Word Order Typology Interacts With Linguistic Complexity: A Cross-Linguistic Corpus Study
    Yadav, Himanshu
    Vaidya, Ashwini
    Shukla, Vishakha
    Husain, Samar
    [J]. COGNITIVE SCIENCE, 2020, 44 (04)
  • [3] Cross-linguistic conditions on word length
    Wichmann, Soren
    Holman, Eric W.
    [J]. PLOS ONE, 2023, 18 (01):
  • [4] Word: A cross-linguistic typology.
    Rice, Curt
    [J]. LANGUAGE, 2006, 82 (04) : 941 - 942
  • [5] Word: A cross-linguistic typology.
    Brown, D
    [J]. JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS, 2004, 40 (02) : 409 - 415
  • [6] A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition
    Bruck, M
    Genesee, F
    Caravolas, M
    [J]. FOUNDATIONS OF READING ACQUISITION AND DYSLEXIA: IMPLICATIONS FOR EARLY INTERVENTION, 1997, : 145 - 162
  • [7] A CROSS-LINGUISTIC STUDY OF EARLY LEXICAL DEVELOPMENT
    CASELLI, MC
    BATES, E
    CASADIO, P
    FENSON, J
    FENSON, L
    SANDERL, L
    WEIR, J
    [J]. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT, 1995, 10 (02) : 159 - 199
  • [8] Cross-linguistic influence, cross-linguistic priming and the nature of shared syntactic structures
    Serratrice, Ludovica
    [J]. LINGUISTIC APPROACHES TO BILINGUALISM, 2016, 6 (06) : 822 - 827
  • [9] Cross-linguistic influence and the MGM
    Grein, Marion
    [J]. LANGUAGE AND DIALOGUE, 2020, 10 (03) : 369 - 388
  • [10] Cross-linguistic activation in bilingual sentence processing: The role of word class meaning
    Baten, Kristof
    Hofman, Fabrice
    Loeys, Tom
    [J]. BILINGUALISM-LANGUAGE AND COGNITION, 2011, 14 (03) : 351 - 359