The effect of a parental mHealth resource on language outcomes in 4- to 5-year-old children

被引:2
|
作者
Le Roux, Elizabeth W. [1 ]
Eccles, Renata [1 ]
Abdoola, Shabnam [1 ]
Graham, Marien [2 ]
van der Linde, Jeannie [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Pretoria, Fac Humanities, Dept Speech Language Pathol & Audiol, Pretoria, South Africa
[2] Univ Pretoria, Fac Educ, Dept Sci Math & Technol Educ, Pretoria, South Africa
关键词
parental resource; mHealth; language development; preschool children; early child development; INTERVENTIONS; TALKING; SKILLS; CHAOS;
D O I
10.4102/sajce.v12i1.1026
中图分类号
G40 [教育学];
学科分类号
040101 ; 120403 ;
摘要
Background: The use of mobile health (mHealth) technology is rapidly expanding in healthcare worldwide. mHealth tools may provide parents with access to resources essential for promoting language development. Aim: The current study aimed to determine how an mHealth parental resource influenced 42 preschool children's (4.0-5.11 months old) language abilities after a 17-week intervention period. Setting: Participants were identified from six early childhood development (ECD) centres from a low-income community in Tshwane, South Africa. Method: A randomised controlled trial (RCT), pre-test post-test research design was employed to determine whether an mHealth parental resource influenced 42 preschool children's (4.05.11 months old) language abilities after a 17-week intervention period. Data were collected using the language subtests of a South African standardised protocol, the Emergent Literacy and Language Assessment Protocol (ELLA). Results: The parental m Health application targeting language stimulation did not significantly improve the experimental group's language outcomes when compared with the control group because most of the parents (n = 27) used the application for less than 20% of the active days. Conclusion: Providing parents with more support with mobile resources may lead to improved usage of the application.
引用
收藏
页数:8
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] Assessment of motor ability of 4- and 5-year-old children
    Oja, L
    Jurimae, T
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF HUMAN BIOLOGY, 1997, 9 (05) : 659 - 664
  • [2] Parental Models and Overimitation in 5-Year-Old Children
    Mackie, Louise
    Eickhoff, Leslie-Ann
    Nimpf, Eluisa
    Huber, Ludwig
    Hoehl, Stefanie
    DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2024,
  • [3] Parental Smoking and Vascular Damage in Their 5-year-old Children
    Geerts, Caroline C.
    Bots, Michiel L.
    van der Ent, Cornelis K.
    Grobbee, Diederick E.
    Uiterwaal, Cuno S. P. M.
    PEDIATRICS, 2012, 129 (01) : 45 - 54
  • [4] Production of temporal terms by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children
    Grant, Janie Busby
    Suddendorf, Thomas
    EARLY CHILDHOOD RESEARCH QUARTERLY, 2011, 26 (01) : 87 - 95
  • [6] Use of context in question answering by 3-, 4- and 5-year-old children
    Ryder, N
    Leinonen, E
    JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC RESEARCH, 2003, 32 (04) : 397 - 415
  • [7] Use of Context in Question Answering by 3-, 4- and 5-Year-Old Children
    Nuala Ryder
    Eeva Leinonen
    Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2003, 32 : 397 - 415
  • [8] SOLUTION OF 2-STIMULUS TRANSPOSITION PROBLEM BY 4- AND 5-YEAR-OLD CHILDREN
    ZEILER, MD
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1966, 71 (04): : 576 - &
  • [9] THE EFFECT OF IMAGINATIVE PLAY ACTIONS ON LANGUAGE-DEVELOPMENT IN 5-YEAR-OLD CHILDREN
    MARBACH, ES
    YAWKEY, TD
    PSYCHOLOGY IN THE SCHOOLS, 1980, 17 (02) : 257 - 263
  • [10] SUCKING HABITS IN 4-YEAR TO 5-YEAR-OLD CHILDREN
    ANKE, B
    JOURNAL OF DENTAL RESEARCH, 1971, 50 (03) : 715 - &