The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation

被引:56
|
作者
Collins, Therese [1 ,2 ]
Wallman, Josh [3 ]
机构
[1] Univ Paris 05, Lab Psychol Percept, F-75006 Paris, France
[2] CNRS UMR 8158, F-75006 Paris, France
[3] CUNY City Coll, Dept Biol, New York, NY 10031 USA
基金
美国国家卫生研究院;
关键词
saccade; plasticity; eye movements; VISUAL ERROR; ADAPTIVE-CONTROL; EYE-MOVEMENTS; MEMORY; SIZE; SIGNALS;
D O I
10.1152/jn.00746.2011
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
Collins T, Wallman J. The relative importance of retinal error and prediction in saccadic adaptation. J Neurophysiol 107: 3342-3348, 2012. First published March 21, 2012; doi:10.1152/jn.00746.2011.-When saccades systematically miss their visual target, their amplitude adjusts, causing the position errors to be progressively reduced. Conventionally, this adaptation is viewed as driven by retinal error (the distance between primary saccade endpoint and visual target). Recent work suggests that the oculomotor system is informed about where the eye lands; thus not all "retinal error" is unexpected. The present study compared two error signals that may drive saccade adaptation: retinal error and prediction error (the difference between predicted and actual postsaccadic images). Subjects made saccades to a visual target in two successive sessions. In the first session, the target was extinguished during saccade execution if the amplitude was smaller (or, in other experiments, greater) than the running median, thereby modifying the average retinal error subjects experienced without moving the target during the saccade as in conventional adaptation paradigms. In the second session, targets were extinguished at the start of saccades and turned back on at a position that reproduced the trial-by-trial retinal error recorded in the first session. Despite the retinal error in the first and second sessions having been identical, adaptation was severalfold greater in the second session, when the predicted target position had been changed. These results argue that the eye knows where it lands and where it expects the target to be, and that deviations from this prediction drive saccade adaptation more strongly than retinal error alone.
引用
收藏
页码:3342 / 3348
页数:7
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] Saccadic adaptation without retinal postsaccadic error
    Bonnetblanc, Francois
    Baraduc, Pierre
    NEUROREPORT, 2007, 18 (13) : 1399 - 1402
  • [2] Variance of visual error influences saccadic adaptation
    Havermann, K.
    Lappe, M.
    PERCEPTION, 2009, 38 : 183 - 183
  • [3] Decoding Trans-Saccadic Prediction Error
    Barne, Louise Catheryne
    Giordano, Jonathan
    Collins, Therese
    Desantis, Andrea
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2023, 43 (11): : 1933 - 1939
  • [4] Saccadic gain modification: Visual error drives motor adaptation
    Wallman, J
    Fuchs, AF
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 1998, 80 (05) : 2405 - 2416
  • [5] Relative-error prediction
    Park, H
    Stefanski, LA
    STATISTICS & PROBABILITY LETTERS, 1998, 40 (03) : 227 - 236
  • [6] Postsaccadic eye position contributes to oculomotor error estimation in saccadic adaptation
    Gremmler, Svenja
    Lappe, Markus
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 2019, 122 (05) : 1909 - 1917
  • [7] Sensitivity to prediction error in reach adaptation
    Marko, Mollie K.
    Haith, Adrian M.
    Harran, Michelle D.
    Shadmehr, Reza
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 2012, 108 (06) : 1752 - 1763
  • [8] Temporal characteristics of error signals driving saccadic gain adaptation in the macaque monkey
    Shafer, JL
    Noto, CT
    Fuchs, AF
    JOURNAL OF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY, 2000, 84 (01) : 88 - 95
  • [9] Trans-saccadic adaptation of perceived size independent of saccadic adaptation
    Bosco, Annalisa
    Rifai, Katharina
    Wahl, Siegfried
    Fattori, Patrizia
    Lappe, Markus
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2020, 20 (07):
  • [10] Saccadic adaptation in children
    Salman, Michael S.
    Sharpe, James A.
    Eizenman, Moshe
    Lillakas, Linda
    To, Teresa
    Westall, Carol
    Dennis, Maureen
    Steinbach, Martin J.
    JOURNAL OF CHILD NEUROLOGY, 2006, 21 (12) : 1025 - 1031