When Two Heads Are Better Than One and When They Can Be Worse: The Amplification Hypothesis

被引:35
|
作者
Koriat, Asher [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Haifa, Dept Psychol, IL-3498838 Haifa, Israel
关键词
group decisions; subjective confidence; wisdom of crowds; self-consistency model; consensual amplification; GROUP DECISION-MAKING; SUBJECTIVE CONFIDENCE; ATTITUDE CERTAINTY; SOCIAL-INFLUENCE; JUDGMENTS; WISDOM; MEMORY; PERFORMANCE; CROWD; ACCURACY;
D O I
10.1037/xge0000092
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
According to the self-consistency model (Koriat, 2012a), confidence judgments in the responses to 2-alternative forced-choice items are correlated with the consensuality of the responses rather than with their correctness: For consensually correct (CC) items, for which the majority response is correct, accuracy is higher for the correct answer than for the wrong answer, whereas for consensually wrong items (CW), confidence is higher for the wrong answer. Assuming that group decisions are dominated by the more confident members, a maximum confidence slating (MCS) algorithm that was applied to virtual dyads outperformed the better member for CC items, but yielded worse performance than the worse member for CW items (Koriat, 2012b). We examined whether group deliberation also amplifies the tendencies that are exhibited by individual decisions, or rather improves performance for both CC and CW items. A perceptual task and a general-information task yielded very similar results. MCS applied to the individual decisions yielded a similar amplification as in Koriat (2012b), but dyadic interaction accentuated this amplification further. Thus, group deliberation had an added effect over confidence-based judgments, possibly due to the exchange of arguments within a dyad, but both confidence slating and group deliberation affected performance in the same direction, improving accuracy when individual accuracy was better than chance, but impairing it when individual accuracy was below chance. Notably, for CW items, group interaction not only impaired accuracy but also enhanced confidence in the erroneous decisions. The mechanisms underlying consensual amplifications were discussed.
引用
收藏
页码:934 / 950
页数:17
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] When two heads are not better than one
    Jerrold, Laurance
    [J]. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF ORTHODONTICS AND DENTOFACIAL ORTHOPEDICS, 2009, 135 (01) : 133 - 135
  • [2] When Are Two Heads Better than One and Why?
    Koriat, Asher
    [J]. SCIENCE, 2012, 336 (6079) : 360 - 362
  • [3] When two heads are better than one expert
    Wiley, J
    Jolly, C
    [J]. PROCEEDINGS OF THE TWENTY-FIFTH ANNUAL CONFERENCE OF THE COGNITIVE SCIENCE SOCIETY, PTS 1 AND 2, 2003, : 1242 - 1246
  • [4] WHEN 2 HEADS ARE BETTER THAN ONE
    ABBIS, JA
    [J]. LASER FOCUS WORLD, 1995, 31 (03): : 59 - 59
  • [5] When two is worse than one
    M. Teresa Villanueva
    [J]. Nature Reviews Clinical Oncology, 2011, 8 (5) : 252 - 252
  • [6] Keyicine: When two heads are better than one to identify an antibiotic !
    Carignan, Alex
    Fortier, Louis-Charles
    [J]. M S-MEDECINE SCIENCES, 2018, 34 (05): : 377 - 379
  • [7] Are two (myosin) heads better (or worse) than one?
    White, HD
    Zhang, XZ
    Belknap, B
    Cartwright, S
    [J]. BIOPHYSICAL JOURNAL, 2001, 80 (01) : 79A - 79A
  • [8] Confidence in individual and group decision making: When "two heads" are worse than one
    Puncochar, JM
    Fox, PW
    [J]. JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 96 (03) : 582 - 591
  • [9] Recognising faces seen alone or with others: When two heads are worse than one
    Megreya, Ahmed M.
    Burton, A. Mike
    [J]. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 20 (07) : 957 - 972
  • [10] When two is better than one
    Babbitt, Courtney C.
    Haygood, Ralph
    Wray, Gregory A.
    [J]. CELL, 2007, 131 (02) : 225 - 227